There are a lot of questions about the validity of IQ tests and the nature of ‘intelligence’.
Ken Richardson, author of Understanding Intelligence tries to tackle the problem at the heart of the subject of intelligence by putting intelligence in the context of living functions.
The subject
of intelligence has been dominated by psychologists using IQ tests. They are
collections of short questions and puzzles, selected because performances on
them (as overall scores) correlate with educational and social criteria that
are themselves assumed to be “measures” of intelligence. There is
little other theory involved. That leaves a lot of questions about the validity
of the tests and the nature of intelligence.
More importantly it leaves a theoretical vacuum inside the whole subject of intelligence. Understanding Intelligence tries to tackle that problem by putting intelligence in the context of living functions in general. After critically dismissing the concept of IQ, and the nature-nurture debate around it, attention is drawn to the origins of life itself. It shows that living systems – able to survive and maintain integrity across changing environments – could only have arisen as ‘intelligent systems’. By that is meant able to anticipate future states by registering how changes in one variable is associated with (depends upon) changes in others, at different levels (as with driving in busy traffic: across numerous variables, signals and manoeuvres are always context-dependent). By assimilating deep statistical dependencies self-organising molecular mixes were able to mutually compensate for, or even anticipate, changes caused by changes in their environments. Such abilities are now being regularly reported, even in single cells.
The book
argues that such intelligent systems became the real basis of the evolutionary
fountain biologists report, starting from single cells. A famous example is the
sporadic cooperation between otherwise solitary amoeba into ‘social’ bodies
called slime moulds. The patterns of signals between cells, rapidly changing
over space and time, not only directs individual motion, within cells. Along
with feedforward/feedback loops they give rise to a new intelligent system
between them, sometimes referred to as the ‘language’ or ‘cognition’ of slime
moulds.
Multicellular
organisms could not have evolved without such systems of intelligent
communication and action. They reproduce from a single fertilized egg, from
which develops hundreds of different cells. As they differentiate they arrive
in just the right place in the developing embryo at just the right time. They
all have the same genes, so they can only ‘know’ how to do that through the
space- and time-dependent signals between them.
In more
evolved species, cells became tissues to form distinct organs. Local
communication between cells continues. But it is now nested within a wider,
hierarchical system we call physiology. That makes the whole much more
sensitive to changes in the outside world, and able to coordinate responses
within for the general good. We now know how physiology is an emergent
intelligent system, functioning through the interdependencies of signals at
many levels, with numerous feedforward/feedback loops, and the predictably
engendered.
Increasing
sensitivity and responsiveness to rapid change in the outside world fostered
behaviour. But that needed nervous systems and then brains, and the cognitive
functions we most think of as intelligence. These have evolved as
amplifications of physiology. Instead of signals being somewhat haphazardly
‘broadcast’, they can be routed down long fibres to special connections
(synapses) on cell clusters, the beginnings of brains. Even in the brains of
insects and spiders, with a few hundred to a few thousand brain cells, a
staggering depth of statistical structure can be captured for predictability
and behavior. More advanced systems have millions of cells connecting with
thousands of others through billions of connections. With the enormous
plasticity of connections, changes in the world are continuously tracked,
dependencies learned, and futures anticipated.
The thing is
that those patterns, transcending mere input-output streams, are what we call
cognition – a new intelligent system, emergent from precursors, for dealing
with more rapid and complex change and creating novel responses to it. These
patterns themselves interact, and new ones emerge in the process, with
staggering depths of statistical structure. In that way, the world constructed
by the brain, “inside”, is far more complex and informative than that
directly experienced.
With such
behaviours, brains and cognitive systems, individual bodies themselves could
interact and cooperate. The amazing constructiveness of ants and bees is often
referred to as ‘swarm’ intelligence. But the three vital ingredients are clear:
a signaling or communication system (here, mostly chemical ‘pheromones’);
feedforward/feedback loops; and the emergence of patterns of statistical
dependencies.
That social
intelligence amounts to a further, evolved, intelligent system. Later, in the
evolution of flocks, shoals and herds of animals it relied increasingly on
bigger brains. As more complex arrays of variables needed to be comprehended,
social intelligence needed still bigger brains. The tendency reached a kind of
peak in monkeys and apes, and much has been made of their human-like
characteristics and cognitive abilities. But real cooperation among individuals
in their groups is limited.
There is
little doubt that even the early ancestors of humans were cooperative hunters
as a significant part of their way of life, millions of years ago. Cooperative
hunting places unprecedented demands on cognitive systems: deeper dependencies
across many more variables: targets rapidly moving, in different and constantly
changing directions; perceptions and responses needing to be tuned to those of
other group members over space and time; integrating streams of visual, vocal
and other cues; and so on. In sum, a more complex intelligence between brains
became demanded to regulate what goes on within them.
It constitutes a new, and unique, intelligent system, a socio-cognitive one. By analogy with the ‘language’ of slime moulds, or of ant colonies, it entails emergent statistical structures between members, only now actualised in shared symbolic forms: the grammar of a unique language; a myriad hardware and social and cognitive “tools” fashioned for sharing and collaboration: hammers, visible and audible cues and gestures, emblems, laws, customs, marriage rules, and so on, and so on. This is what we call human culture.
The important point is that culture is more than merely copying habits from others, as many evolutionary psychologists have assumed. In the constant interaction between and within brains, and many feedforward/feedback loops, novel cultural tools are being constantly created. They both deal with environmental change and change it in return, as modern science and technology emphatically demonstrate. The consequence is that, unlike all other species, humans adapt the world to themselves rather than vice versa. The story of life is truly the story of intelligence.
Latest Comments
Have your say!