Standardized tests are one of those topics that many people
have an opinion about, despite most people being uninformed. Memories of
filling in bubble sheets during childhood or anxiety about college admissions
tests color people’s perceptions. Additionally, the highly technical field of
test development and the confidentiality surrounding test content (to prevent
cheating) makes standardized tests seem like a “black box.” As a result, many
incorrect beliefs have sprung up about these tests.
One of the most common incorrect beliefs about tests is that
they are biased against racial and ethnic minorities. It is easy to see where
this belief comes from. In many countries, marginalized and underrepresented
groups have lower average scores than the averages found in politically,
socially, and economically dominant groups. Despite the tremendous amount of
overlap in scores across groups, the differences between averages is often a
noticeable characteristic of score reports. A reasonable explanation for many
members of the media and the public is that something must be wrong with the
tests and that they must be bias towards minorities.
The reality is more complex than this. Differences in
average scores can occur for many reasons, one of which is bias in the test.
Psychologists have studied test bias for over 50 years, and today they have
many tools for investigating test bias.
One important result of the research on test bias is a
fundamental understanding of what, exactly, test bias is. Whereas laymen
understand average score differences as being evidence of test bias, the
technical definition among experts is more complex. For the testing industry, bias
occurs when two people from different demographic groups—who are equal in
ability—earn different scores on a test because of their demographic group
membership. Thus, identifying test bias requires looking for differences that
emerge in these groups of equal ability, who speak the same native language,
are acculturated to the country, etc.
This is not to say that average score differences are
irrelevant. Indeed, they are often a good indicator that the psychologists
should investigate whether test bias is present. But—by themselves—they are not
sufficient to show that a test is actually biased.
So how common is test bias in professionally developed
tests? The answer is that it is almost non-existent. Today, it is routine for
professionally developed tests to be screened for bias before they are sold or
administered on a large scale, and doing so is part of the field’s ethics code
and professional standards. Moreover, the underlying cognitive architecture of
intelligence and related abilities seems remarkably stable across cultures,
thus making it easier to create a test that can function for multiple groups.
Unfortunately, what psychologists have known for decades about test bias has not filtered to the general public. As a result, tests often come under attack because well-meaning outsiders see them as an engine of unfairness. However, an unbiased test can be a tool to strengthen society because it can be a barometer of progress and identify individual excellence in people from all backgrounds.
Latest Comments
Have your say!